首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   77篇
  免费   5篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   1篇
  2018年   5篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   3篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   5篇
  2011年   2篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   12篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
排序方式: 共有84条查询结果,搜索用时 203 毫秒
61.
In anonymous game tasks, individuals’ prosocial behaviour was shown to increase when those individuals were provided with social cues from a third party or bystander. It has been suggested that those social cues can be presented using a configuration suggestive of a face with ‘watching eyes’. This led us to question whether it was possible to provide the watching‐eyes configuration with even weaker facial information, such as a simple dot pattern. By using a minimal social cue paradigm, such as the one described above, the present research found that individuals’ trust toward the trustee increased when they detected the watching‐eyes dot pattern as a source of social cues in two settings: a hypothetical investment game for both college students (Study 1) and customs officers (Study 2); and an actual investment game for college students (Study 3).  相似文献   
62.
Acquisition and maintenance of trusting behavior   总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
This study determined whether a two-person exchange situation contained natural contingencies for trusting behavior or whether external contingencies were necessary. Pairs of college students worked matching-to-sample problems for money. On each trial there was one problem and the subjects determined which of them would solve it. Trusting behavior was defined as an increase in the number of consecutive problems each subject allowed his partner to work during sessions that also ended with an equitable distribution. Simply, trust was a temporary deviation from equity. A subject could give the problem to the other person (cooperate), or not respond and let the other person take the problem (share). Other possibilities were for both subjects to try to take the problem (complete), or for neither subject to respond and thereby let the person who worked the last problem also work the next one (passive trust). When only four lever pulls were required to distribute a problem (no external contingencies to reach either equity or trust) subjects reached equity, but only minimal trust (strict alternation of single problems) developed in 18 sessions. When 30 or 60 lever pulls were required to distribute a problem (smaller response requirement for passive trust and therefore a contingency for trust), trusting behavior developed after a few sessions (fixed ratio 30) or after several trials of the first session (fixed ratio 60) and it ordinarily expanded gradually to 10 to 15 consecutive problems through passive trust. The aversiveness of the inequity involved in trusting appears to necessitate a contingency for acquisition. Once trust develops, however, this aversiveness is reduced as subjects learn the inequity is only temporary (e.g., once trust was acquired at fixed ratio 60 it was maintained at fixed ratio 4, which would not initially produce it), and the direction of the inequity appears to become of questionable importance (e.g., being behind was alternated over rather than within sessions and usually not in a systematic manner).  相似文献   
63.
施动感是个体在自主运动过程中对自身行为产生的主体性体验,揭示其发展过程对了解自我意识的形成具有重要意义。三阶段模式全面概括了施动感的发展过程。婴儿形成动作预测、建立动作-效果联结以及与养育者良好的人际互动对施动感的发展起着重要作用。未来施动感发展研究应从多种施动感生成理论出发,改进婴幼儿施动感的测量范式,探索施动感发展与社会认知发展、自闭症症状的关联。  相似文献   
64.
Novelty detection is essential to adapt to changes. However, the relationship between novelty detection and visual recognition memory remains unclear. To characterize the temporal dynamics of novelty and its connection to familiarity, we probed early behavioural performance of novelty and familiarity in 31 participants using a speeded go/no-go recognition task with a 600-ms response deadline. Responses to familiarity and novelty produced symmetrical biases and correlated accuracies and biases, but novelty decisions were less accurate and had slower minimal reaction times (410?ms). These processes thus appear to be independent, as suggested by a more efficient system in the case of familiarity, but with common factors bringing overlapping contributions to both processes. This may possibly be explained by the more fluent processing of repeated stimuli, but with familiarity and novelty potentially relying on one decision criterion, as suggested by the correlated and remarkably symmetrical biases. This study supports models that conceptualize novelty and familiarity decisions as two partly overlapping processes.  相似文献   
65.
Wynne的简约模型是基于动物的传递性关系推理研究所提出的理论模型。该模型假设:个体的传递性关系推理是通过运用刺激的强化值来表征顺序信息,并比较所呈现刺激之间强化值的差异来实现的。研究文献证实简约模型能够说明在人类和其它动物的传递性关系推理研究中所发现的大多数标准化效应,因此其被视为迄今解释传递性关系推理问题最经济的模型。但该模型将人类高级认知活动采用动物的机械学习方式来解释的还原论倾向是值得商榷的,它所探讨的传递性行为是否是“真传递性关系推理”也有待进一步验证。  相似文献   
66.
Bo Mou 《Metaphilosophy》2000,31(3):262-286
In this paper, I give a metaphilosophical analysis of the core idea of deflationism by discussing some basic conceptual and methodological issues involved in the debate between deflationism and substantivism. In so doing, I argue for three positive points. First, the crux of the dispute between deflationism and substantivism is whether or not truth is substantive in its metaphysical nature and in its explanatory role in philosophical enterprises, rather than whether or not a minimal approach regarding conceptual resources is taken to explain truth; a minimal approach itself is philosophically innocent. Second, there is no intrinsic connection between the core idea of deflationism and the Tarski-style equivalence thesis, which is often identified as implying or supporting the former. Third, there are some unbridged fundamental gaps between the core idea of deflationism and various redundancy theses; these redundancy theses cannot be identified as the former, nor can they be used to justify the former on their own.  相似文献   
67.
回顾性分析乳腺浸润性微乳头状癌(IMPC)的临床病理特征.选取河北医科大学第四医院乳腺中心手术切除并经病理证实的女性原发性乳腺癌共3 383例,筛选出符合IMPC为实验组,同期乳腺浸润性导管癌(IDC)为对照组.结果显示,本组IMPC 167例,占同期乳腺癌4.94%,年龄29岁~80岁,中位年龄54岁.淋巴结转移者共135例,占80.8%,淋巴结转移数目≥4个共105例,占62.9%,HER2阳性率58.7%,与IDC组相比差异均有统计学意义(P<0.05).IMPC为一种少见类型的浸润性乳腺癌,具有淋巴管侵袭性强、淋巴结转移率高的生物学行为.应与其他类型肿瘤进行鉴别,明确诊断.  相似文献   
68.
为了比较幕上高血压脑出血传统开颅与微创治疗的效果及其并发症,并对其手术时机进行探讨,对30例传统开颅组和104例微创治疗组进行对比研究。结果显示,术后6个月微创治疗组致残率及肺部感染发生率较传统开颅组明显为低(P0.05);早期(8h~24h)手术死亡率较超早期(8h)明显为低(P0.05)。因此,微创治疗能够有效降低高血压脑出血患者的致残率和术后并发症的发生率,其手术时机适宜选择在早期。  相似文献   
69.
探讨B超配合在麦默通微创行乳腺良性肿块切除术中的作用。对569例乳腺肿块进行麦默通微创切除术,分别于术前进行B超定位,术中进行B超引导,术后B超探查及随访,分析B超配合对手术的彻底性、安全性和准确性等所起的作用与意义。结果569例患者的乳腺病灶均被准确切除,B超熟练程度决定手术的准确性、彻底性及手术时间。术中B超配合20次以上者,其一次定位成功并一次完全切除率达87.4%,与初次(B超配合20次以下)配合者相比,两者有显著性差异(P=0.000);手术时间亦比初次B超配合者要短,两者有显著性差异(P=0.000)。熟练的B超配合,有利于麦默通微创旋切技术准确、有效、快速而安全的切除乳腺良性肿瘤。  相似文献   
70.
This paper discusses the residues of a somatic countertransference that revealed its meaning several years after apparently successful analytic work had ended. Psychoanalytic and Jungian analytic ideas on primitive communication, dissociation and enactment are explored in the working through of a shared respiratory symptom between patient and analyst. Growth in the analyst was necessary so that the patient's communication at a somatic level could be understood. Bleger's concept that both the patient's and analyst's body are part of the setting was central in the working through.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号